Table Of ContentWord-order and the Interpretation of Nominals in Plains Cree
Jeff Mühlbauer
1. Introduction1
Until recently, Algonquian linguistic research has not considered word order and
phrase structure to be particularly important components of these languages. The decision
to exclude phrase structure from discussion is based on the seemingly erratic distribution
of constituents in an Algonquian utterance when the traditional Algonquianist categories
of noun, verb, and particle (i.e. everything that is not a noun or a verb) are the sole focus
of study. This characterization is born out when a typical sentence is tested with native
speakers (from Déchaine 1997).
1) a. wâpamê-w, nâpêw, êkoni atimw-a. VSO
see-3, man, this dog-obv
b. wâpamêw, êkoni atimwa, nâpêw. VOS
c. nâpêw, wâpamêw êkoni atimwa. SVO
d. êkoni atimwa, wâpamêw nâpêw. OVS
e. nâpêw, êkoni atimwa, wâpamêw. SOV
f. êkoni atimwa, nâpêw, wâpamêw. OSV
The three Algonquianist categories of noun, verb, and particle (i.e. everything that is not
a noun or a verb) indeed occur in any possible combination, and are at least superficially
equivalent in the syntax. Word order does not appear to affect the assignment of
grammatical functions. Thus, in a strictly predicational sense, Algonquian languages have
"free" word order.
However, there is strong evidence to suggest that these word orders are not
equivalent in the semantics. In particular, the position of a nominal within the sentence is
related to its interpretation, and has distinct consequences both for the semantics and the
syntax. This can be demonstrated by tracking the position of nominals in large contextual
spans, followed up with carefully-structured elicitation.
Because this particular area of Algonquian linguistics is so poorly-studied, there is
a great deal of work to be done. To that end, this paper is intended to be the outline of a
research program in the semantic consequences of nominal position, with special focus
on Plains Cree.
1 This work is supported in part through SSHRC grant (SSHRC 412-97-0016) to Anna-Marie DiSciullo and
R.-M. Déchaine. Special thanks to Wally Awâsis, Clare Cook, Henry Davis, Rose-Marie Déchaine, Diana
Gibraiel, Lisa Matthewson, and H.C. Wolfart. All mistakes are my own, and, as Al Gore once said of the
Constitution, this paper is a living document.
Key: 21=1pl Inclusive, OBV = obviative, ACC = nominative/accusative marker, ERG = ergative/absolutive
marker, AS/IS =animate/inanimate subject, AO/IO=animate/inanimate object, conj=conjunct clause marker,
REL=relative clause marker, NEG = negation, Q = question, STAT = stative, DIM = diminutive, POSS =
possessor theme marker, USC = unspecified subject construction. JKN = Jim Kâ-Nîpitêhtêw Text. SW1 =
Sarah Whitecalf's first lecture.
1
1.1 Linguist-produced Sentences vs. Native Speaker-Produced Sentences
Many native speakers of Plains Cree have amazing flexibility in interpreting
constituent structure (see §1). Given multiple constituents, many word orders can be
made acceptable. Thus, if we are to begin a careful characterization of word order effects,
a sharp line is needed between sentences produced by the linguist and those produced by
the consultant.
Linguists have used two different methods of testing sentence structure – texts
and elicitation, with widely different results. This variation should not be surprising,
since a text constitutes the uninterrupted discourse of a fluent speaker, whereas elicitation
can involve linguists who do not necessarily understand the complete architecture of the
language.
Interestingly, freedom of word-order is not equally demonstrated by the linguist
and the consultant. Although it is true that many word orders are deemed acceptable by
speakers, the variety of word orders produced by the native speaker in a natural context
shows a great deal less variation. Thus, for the purposes of this study, I will be
preferential to sentences produced by native speakers without interference from linguists.
Until we understand a great deal more about word order effects than we currently do, this
can be the only reliable course of action.
1.1 Survey of the Field
In his 1962 grammar of Menominee (an Algonquian language spoken in Wisconsin),
Bloomfield summarizes Menominee's phrase structure in this way:
The description of phrase structure is rendered partly indeterminate by stylistic variations
of various sorts. The constituents of a phrase often appear in other than the usual order, or
separated by other words, or with pause intonation between them, or with two or all three
of these. (Bloomfield 1962:440)
Wolfart does not discuss phrase structure at all in his 1973 grammar of Plains Cree, nor
does Uhlenbeck in his 1938 grammar of Blackfoot. Thus some of the original major
works on the western branch of Algonquian languages have nothing of use to say on the
subject of word order.
As Algonquian linguistics developed in the 1990s, several scholars began to
seriously consider issues of word order. Ahenakew (1987) details the unique uses of the
demonstrative ôma in Plains Cree, which shows strong ordering restrictions. Tomlin and
Rhodes (1992) considered Ojibwa narratives, looking at the distribution of words, and
determined that the neutral or "unmarked" order for Ojibwa was verb initial. Dahlstrom
(1995) deals with topic and focus structure in Algonquian, determining that there is a
topic position on the left edge of the verbal complex, followed by a modifier position.
Finally, Wolfart (1996) comments on the intonational pauses in Plains Cree, and what
they might mean for Plains Cree word order. Most recently, Déchaine et. al. (2003) has
considered the ordering of modifiers, and Shields (2003) has looked at discourse roles of
nominals and their ordering in Menominee.
2
In the broader world of linguistics, work on word order has been more prolific.
Givón (1979) draws a distinction between word order possibilities in languages by
characterizing a set of languages (e.g. Latin and Greek) that are "pragmatically-
conditioned" in their word order. In his work on Walpiri, Hale coined the term "non-
configurational" to characterize languages whose word-order is free. Baker (1996) and
Jelinek (1984) have sought to encode this characterization in modern syntactic theory by
positing that these languages introduce their arguments as morphology on the verb head,
and thus all other DPs are adjuncts. In 1992, Hale refined his definition of "free" word
order strictly to describe languages that do not have word order affect grammatical
function.
1.2 Limits to Word Order Flexbility
Despite the generalization repeated above, it is not entirely true that word order
does not affect grammatical function. This is demonstrable in the work of several
linguists, as well as my own fieldwork, and bears on the central concern of this paper –
word-ordering of nominals – by showing that word-ordering touches many diverse areas
of the grammar.
Ahenakew (1987) and Déchaine (1997) consider the predicational use of
demonstratives, which shows extremely strict word-order restrictions.
2) a. ôma maskisin b. maskisin ôma
this shoe shoe this
"this shoe" "This is a shoe" (Ahenakew 1987)
Here, the position of the determiner with respect to the nominal has serious consequences
for grammatical function. The prenominal demonstrative creates a DP, while the
postnominal demonstrative creates a predicational structure. Further, this construction is
productive in clefting contexts.
3) a. "êwak ôma kâ-mâcîhtâ-yân ôta,"
topic this REL-begin-1 here,
"this which I began here" (JKN 46.2)
b. "êkota ôma wîst ê-atoskêt ôta,"
there this 3-emph.PR CONJ-work-3 here
"It is there that she also works," (JKN 46.29)
Here, the clefting is used with a topic marker and a locative, and is crucial to maintenance
and shifting of topics (see §4.2.1 below). This predication structure can even be used to
cleft larger constituents, in what Ahenakew (1987) calls a "factitive" use of the
demonstrative.
4) "kîkway anima k-âta-~-kakwê-kitâsômâyahk, êkâ kîkway ka-tôtahk anima,"
"kîkway anima k-âta-~-kakwê-kitâsô-m-â-yahk, êkâ kîkway ka-tôt-am-k anima
something this chng-although-try.to-warn-by.mouth-ACC-21 NEG something REL-thus-
3
do-IO-3 this
"it is that we have tried to warn them against something in vain, that they do not do
something," (JKN 46.24-25)
Here, the entire verb structure, along with negation and an indefinite pronoun, is in
predicate position, with the distal demonstrative anima creating the predicational
structure. These kinds of restrictions have also been discussed by Wolvengrey (2003).
Some other data that bears on this issue comes from contexts that use overt
nominals and adverbs. Given a verb, a noun, and an adverb, a native speaker can accept
most word orders, with an interesting exception (from elicitation).
5) a. ni-npa-h-â-w ohcitaw amiskw
1-sleep-CAUS-ACC-3 on.purpose beaver
"I killed a beaver on purpose"
b. # ni-npa-h-â-w amisk ohcitaw
1-sleep-CAUSE-ACC-3 on.purpose beaver
"I killed the beaver, who was a beaver on purpose."
This second ordering can be rescued by right-dislocating the adverb with a pause.
6) ni-npa-h-â-w amiskw, ohcitaw
1-sleep-CAUSE-ACC-3 beaver, on.purpose
"I killed a beaver on purpose."
Thus word-ordering plays a crucial role in the resolution of adverbial modification. This
finding is consistent with what Déchaine et. al. (2003) found in their study of modifier
positioning.
Other phenomena that show syntactic word-ordering restrictions include relative
clauses, clefting constructions, reason clauses, and construct-state possession. An account
of Plains Cree that assumes syntactically free word-ordering could not account for any
these phenomena, since the grammatical functions crucially depend on the ordering of the
constituents. Thus, despite the generalization that word order is usually "free" with
respect to syntax, we should be aware that this, like many generalizatoins in Plains Cree,
is not entirely true.
2 Macro-Syntax vs. Micro-Syntax
Before going in to an account of the semantics and syntax of nominal positioning,
it is important to consider what components of Plains Cree grammar are potentially
accessible to operations that could impact word order, such as scrambling, extraction,
left-dislocation, raising, etc. There are essentially two domains in Algonquian languages
that are relevant to a syntactic and semantic analysis. For the purposes of this study, I will
tentatively term these two domains "Macro-Syntax" and "Micro-Syntax".
4
2.1 Micro-Syntax
The first domain to consider is Micro-Syntax. The ordering of constituents within
this domain is generally rigid, allowing only specific local reorderings (see Cook 2003 on
semantic and syntactic consequences of preverb ordering). In contrast to Macro-Syntax,
these components are not able to be disconnected from one another. For example, when
asking a question about an event in Plains Cree, the entire verbal complex must be
fronted as a single constituent.
7) ki-kî-wâp-m-â-w cî ana nâpêw * kî cî kiwâpamâw ana nâpêw
2-perf-see-by.eye-ACC-3 Q that man
kiwâpamâw cî ana nâpêw
"Did you see that man?"
Likewise, in relative clauses, extracting the nominal head does not affect the verb's
pronominal marking.
8) ni-kî-wâp-m-â-w ana nâpêw kâ-ahko-si-t * ana nâpêw kâ-ahkosi_
1-PERF-see-by.eye-ACC-3 that man REL-sick-STAT-3
nkîwâpmâw ana nâpêw kâhkosit
"I saw the man who was sick."
This is in sharp contrast to languages like English, which allow the extraction of a single
functional component (e.g. tense with do-support).
The domain of Micro-Syntax encompasses all head-marking nominal and verbal
morphology. For nominals, this includes pronominal marking, plural and obviation
marking, non-verbal adjectives, and the possessor marker, as shown in 9.
9) [PERS [PREN1 [PREN2 [NOUN] IM] DIM]STEM PERSNUM] NUM]2
ki-wâpiski-pôsîs-im-sîs-inaw-ak
2-white-cat-poss-dim-21pl-pl
kiwâpskipôsimsisnawak
"our white kittens"
For verbs, Micro-Syntax includes impersonal marking, a subset of aspectual and modal
markers, transitivity suffixes, incorporated nouns, ergative/accusative marking, and so
on.3 This is all in addition to the pronominal marking seen on nouns, making for a
syntactically complex structure.
10) [PERS [PREV* [ROOT] MANNER] ERG/ACC] PERS] NUM]
STEM
ni-kî-wâp-m-â-w-ak
1-perf-see-by.eye-acc-3-pl
2 For the reasoning behind 2 prenoun positions, see Mühlbauer (2003b).
3 See Wolfart (1973) for a full treatment of all nominal and verbal morphology.
5
nkîwâpmâwak
"I have seen them."
Several linguists have attempted to account for the structure of these Micro-
Syntactic components, including Déchaine and Reinholz (1997), Déchaine (1999) and
Hirose (2000). In these accounts, the focus has been on the specific syntactic functions
that these morphemes have. For example, Hirose (2000) shows that there is a one-to-one
correspondence between syntactic arguments and stem-internal verbal suffixes. Therefore
these morphemes correlate with transitivity marking, which translates to a vP shell in a
syntactic analysis like Kratzer (1993).
11) wâpa-m-ê-w
"he sees him"
vP
V
pro vP
V
v vP
-m- V
pro vP
V
v
-ê-
Déchaine (1999) deals with the question of linear-ordering of these elements by
analyzing it as a case of mixed-affixation. A noun, for example, moves through a series
of spec positions and checks features off. These higher heads attach to the noun via
phrasal encliticization, either as prefixes or suffixes.
12)
DP
V
D PERSP
V
PERS NUMP
V
NUM NP
V
N
Thus, in these analyses, Micro-Syntax reduces to phonologically-dependent morphemes
that carry simplex syntactic content.
Note that some of the syntactic structure spelled-out in Plains Cree is also
apparent in English, although it is morphologically covert. A verb such as "kick" is
encoded for a certain transitivity and telicity, but is a single morphological component
6
where Plains Cree would have a minimum of three. These covert pieces of "kick" can be
manipulated via modification (e.g. "John was kicked" or "John kicks little children"), but
they cannot be reordered or extracted. Thus the difference between a language like
English and a language like Plains Cree reduces, in some respects, to an issue of
morphological span (c.f. Williams, Déchaine 2003). This means that the existence of
Micro-Syntax is not an Algonquian-specific phenomenon, which would be apparent to
anyone who has tried to determine the meaning of English words like "he", "the", "-ed",
or even "kick" without presenting a speaker with a full, well-formed utterance.
As for the difference in morphological span between Algonquian languages and
other human languages, it is my opinion that this is related to the phonological structure
of these languages (e.g. syllable structure, phonemic inventory, etc). Whether it is the
phonology that causes the syntax or the other way around is well beyond my current
understanding.
2.2 Macro-Syntax
Macro-Syntax operates on the output of Micro-Syntax, such that it manipulates
syntactically-complex constituents as units. Thus, in a DP like Wâpastim o-maskisin-a
"Wâpastim his-shoe-s", Wâpastim and omaskisina can be moved or extracted, but
internal components of either (i.e. Micro-Syntax) can not be. Since this paper is
concerned with the word order effects of nominals, it is Macro-Syntax that will be
central.
If we assume that Plains Cree has a neutral word order for constituents, any
deviation from this neutral order is an instance of Macro-Syntactic operations. This likely
includes question formation (13a), topicalization (13b), dislocation (13c), and rhetorical
strategies like chiasma (13d).
13) a. kikîwâpamâw cî ana nâpêw
2-perf-see-by.eye-AO-3 ques that man
"Did you see that man?"
b. "…kiyânaw tipiyaw kitawâsimisinaw kôsisiminaw, kiyâm pikoyikohk ka-
kisîwêhkahtawâyahk kîkway ê-pakwâtamawâyahk, …"
21 personally 2-child-1pl 2-grandchild-1pl, let.it.be no.matter future-
speak.loudly.at-AO-21 something conj-disapprove.TA-applic-dir-21,
"….our own children and grandchildren, let it be that we speak loudly at
them to disapprove of them for something, …" (JKN 46.21-22)
c. "…tânisi kîkway k-êsi-nahêyihtahk, otawâsimisimâw oyôsisimimâw, …"
how.IPC something.PR chng-thus.RR-have.peace.of.mind.TI-TI-3s, 3-child-poss-dim-
imp-dir-3s 3-grandchild-poss-imp-dir-3s,
"…how they would have some peace of mind, children and grandchildren"
(JKN 46.19-20)
7
d. "…tahk âwiyâk kîkway nikî-ati-miyikwak ê-miywâsik, …"
more.and.more someone something 1-perf-proceed-give-ERG-3-3pl CONJ-good-STAT-3
"and then they gave me more and more of that which is good." (JKN 48.6)
Thus the crucial thing to determine is what the neutral word order is, so that we know
what a deviation is, and therefore what must be accounted for as a Macro-Syntactic
operation. To do this, I will begin by looking at modifier positioning. This will give us
some structure to begin measuring the position of nominals.
3 Modifier Positions
Déchaine, et. al (2003) analyzed a speech by the Plains Cree elder Kâ-
pimwêwêhahk, paying particular attention to the ordering of modifying adjuncts, such as
locative markers like êkota "there", adverbs like wêtinahk "quietly", and temporal
markers like anohc "today". When all occurrences of modifiers were assembled, the
result was a surprisingly structured hierarchy.
14) a. Preverbal b. Postverbal
[TEMP > ADV* > LOC > TEMP > V] [V > ADV/LOC > TEMP]
TEMP TEMP
V V
TEMP ADV* ADV/LOC TEMP
V V
ADV LOC VP ADV/LOC
V
LOC TEMP
V
TEMP VP
Preverbal modifiers are more common, and are more often stacked. Their order is
consistent throughout the text considered. Postverbal modifiers, on the other hand, are
much less common, show limited ordering, and do not allow stacking. This would be
consistent with a verb-fronting analysis, in which the verb raises to some position higher
than one of the adverbial positions.
Predicate modifiers occur internal to topic markers like clefts (c.f. Déchaine 1997)
and other clause-typing elements like negation (c.f. Déchaine and Wiltschko 2002). This
is consistent with Cinque's (1999) hierarchy of functional heads, and provides an
important framework to begin considering where nominals fit in Plains Cree word
ordering.
4 Nominal Positions
Syntactically, the core component of an utterance is the verb. With the exception
of structural cases (Bittner and Hale (1996)), the verb and its functional heads license the
8
existence of DPs and other elements. Semantically, the verb provides the core content of
the sentence, taking an individual and forming a function of individuals to truth
conditions. This centrality gives us a good foothold on the word order of Plains Cree. If
we look at nominals through the lens of their relation to the verb, we can begin to make
generalizations that are reasonably grounded in the semantics and syntax.
4.2 The Preverbal Domain
Preverbal nominals are less common than their postverbal counterparts, and bear
specific functions. This, combined with the previous work done on the ordering patterns
of predicate modifiers, provides us with good motivation to choose preverbal nominals as
the place to begin this description. To this end, I will begin with the leftmost positions in
the preverbal domain and move inwards.
4.2.1 Topic
The notion of "Topic" that I will use for this analysis is tentatively defined as
"what the discourse is about". Since topic is a difficult thing to get one's hands on in any
concrete way, I will make heavy use of a counselling speech by the Plains Cree elder Kâ-
pimwêwêhahk that I have already carefuly dissected for its rhetorical import (c.f.
Déchaine et. al. 2003). This will help me in determining what the discourse is "about".
With this in mind, Plains Cree seems to have what I will call an "inner" and an
"outer" topic. These are differentiated by their position relative to the rest of the clause.
4.2.1.1 Outer Topic
Outer Topics are characterized by left-dislocation effects similar in semantic and
prosodic content to English's left-dislocation phenomenon. Typically, outer topics are
separated from the main clause with an intonation or pause, and refer to a familiar
discourse referent.
15) a. "…kiyânaw tipiyaw kitawâsimisinaw kôsisiminaw, kiyâm pikoyikohk ka-
kisîwêhkahtawâyahk kîkway ê-pakwâtamawâyahk, …"
21 personally 2-child-1pl 2-grandchild-1pl, let.it.be no.matter future-
speak.loudly.at-AO-21 something CONJ-disapprove.TA-applic-dir-21,
"….our own children and grandchildren, let it be that we speak loudly at
them to disapprove of them for something, …" (JKN 46.21-22)
b. "êkosi aniki aniki môniyâsak wîstawâw, 'kiyâm' nika-itwân, 'tita-
kitimâkinâkoyahkok ôma ê-nêhiyâwiyahk,'"
so those those white.man-pl 3.emph.pn, let.it.be 1-fut-say-local, future-respect-
ERG-21-3 this conj-cree-21,
"And for the other Whites, too, 'Let them respect,' I will say, 'that we are Cree,'"
(SW1)
9
c. " - ahpô ayisk môniyâsak, wîstawâw ê-tâpwêwakêyihtahkik êtikwê anima
nipâkwêsimowin,"
or even white.man-PL, 3.emph.pn conj-true-by.mind-IO-3-3PL apparently that
sun.dance-NOM,"
"- for even the Whites themselves must believe in the Sundance," (SW1)
Here, the topic is separated off from the main clause with a pause (marked by Wolfart as
a comma).
The use of left dislocation in all of these examples coincides with a shift in the
topic under discussion. Specifically, the predicate remains notionally constant, but what it
is about (i.e. the topic) has changed. This is a kind of rhetorical parallelism, used to build
a connection between ideas. In 15a, it is a shift from the traditional times of
grandchildren being instructed by the proto-typical old woman to the obstinate
grandchildren of those in the room. In 15b, it is a shift from the specific example of H.C.
Wolfart's respect for Cree ways, to white men in general. In 15c, it is a shift from the
Plains Cree's faith in the Sun Dance to the likelihood of the Whites faith in it. In all these
instances, this new topic had already been under discussion previously, and thus left
dislocation signals a return to an older topic.
4.2.1.2 Inner Topic
Topicalized nominals may also occur as the left-most constituent inside the
clause, preceding clause-typing components like namoya and êka "negation" (c.f.
Déchaine and Wiltschko 2002 for the clausal status of negation), and subjunctive-type
markers like kiyâm "let it be".
16) a. "êwakw âwa ayamihewiyiniw kâ-wî-petât,"
resump this pray-person REL-intend-bring-ACC-3
"that priest is going to bring it," (JKN 50.28)
b. "otôsk-âyima êkâ kwayask ê-isi-wîcêhtoyit."
3-young-person-POSS-OBV NEG properly CONJ-thus-get.along-RECIP-3
"If their young people do not get along with one another." (JKN 48.28)
c. "êwakw ânima pêyak kisêyiniw ê-kî-nakiskawak,"
topic that one old-man conj-perf -meet-by.foot-1>3
"I met a certain old man about that," (JKN 46.6)
Here, the topic is not preposed and separated by a pause, as with Outer Topics.
Inner topic is often used to introduce new discourse referents, who will be the
subject of some span of discourse. Once this introductory topic-marking has been made,
the nominals can be returned to a post-verbal position, as is shown in the beginning of a
sacred story (âtayôhkêwin) told to Bloomfield (1930) by Adam Sâkêwêw.
10